Emotional prosody

Emotional prosody is characterized as an individual's tone of voice in speech that is conveyed through changes in pitch, loudness, timbre, speech rate, and pauses which is different from linguistic and semantic information. It can be isolated from linguistics and interacts with verbal content (e.g. sarcasm). It is perceived or decoded slightly worse than facial expressions but accuracy varies with emotions. Anger and sadness are perceived most easily, followed by fear and happiness, with disgust being the most poorly perceived.[1]

Production of vocal emotion

Studies have found that some emotions, such as fear, joy and anger, are portrayed at a higher frequency than emotions such as sadness.[2]

Perception of vocal emotion

Decoding emotions in speech includes three (3) stages: determining acoustic features, creating meaningful connections with these features, and processing the acoustic patterns in relation to the connections established. In the processing stage, connections with basic emotional knowledge is stored separately in memory network specific to associations. These associations can be used to form a baseline for emotional expressions encountered in the future. Emotional meanings of speech are implicitly and automatically registered after the circumstances, importance and other surrounding details of an event have been analyzed.[4]

On average, listeners are able to perceive intended emotions exhibited to them at a rate significantly better than chance (chance=approximately 10%).[3] However, error rates are also high. This is partly due to the observation that listeners are more accurate at emotional inference from particular voices and perceive some emotions better than others.[2] Vocal expressions of anger and sadness are perceived most easily, fear and happiness are only moderately well-perceived, and disgust has low perceptibility.[1]

The brain in vocal emotions

Language can be split into two components: the verbal and vocal channels. The verbal channel is the semantic content made by the speaker's chosen words. In the verbal channel, the semantic content of the speakers words determines the meaning of the sentence. The way a sentence is spoken however, can change its meaning which is the vocal channel. This channel of language conveys emotions felt by the speaker and gives us as listeners a better idea of the intended meaning. Nuances in this channel are expressed through intonation, intensity, a rhythm which combined for prosody. Usually these channels convey the same emotion, but sometimes they differ. Sarcasm and irony are two forms of humor based on this incongruent style.[5]

Neurological processes integrating verbal and vocal (prosodic) components are relatively unclear. However, it is assumed that verbal content and vocal are processed in different hemispheres of the brain. Verbal content composed of syntactic and semantic information is processed the left hemisphere.Syntactic information is processed primarily in the frontal regions and a small part of the temporal lobe of the brain while semantic information is processed primarily in the temporal regions with a smaller part of the frontal lobes incorporated. In contrast, prosody is processed primarily in the same pathway as verbal content, but in the right hemisphere. Neuroimaging studies using functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI) machines provide further support for this hemisphere lateralization and temporo-frontal activation. Some studies however show evidence that prosody perception is not exclusively lateralized to the right hemisphere and may be more bilateral. There is some evidence that the basal ganglia may also play an important role in the perception of prosody.[5]

Impairment of emotion recognition

It has been found that it gets increasingly difficult to recognize vocal expressions of emotion with increasing age. Older adults have slightly more difficulty labeling vocal expressions of emotion, particularly sadness and anger) than young adults but have a much greater difficulty integrating vocal emotions and corresponding facial expressions. A possible explanation for this difficulty is that combining two sources of emotion requires greater activation of emotion areas of the brain, in which adults show decreased volume and activity. Another possible explanation is that hearing loss could have led to a mishearing of vocal expressions. High frequency hearing loss is known to begin occurring around the age of 50, particularly in men.[6]

Considerations

Most research regarding vocal expression of emotion has been studied through the use of synthetic speech or portrayals of emotion by professional actors. Little research has been done with spontaneous, "natural" speech samples. These artificial speech samples have been considered to be close to natural speech but specifically portrayals by actors may be influenced stereotypes of emotional vocal expression and may exhibit intensified characteristics of speech skewing listeners perceptions. Another consideration lies in listeners individual perceptions. Studies typically take the average of responses but few examine individual differences in great depth. This may provide a better insight into the vocal expressions of emotions.[3]

References

  1. 1 2 "The Social and Emotional Voice" (PDF). Retrieved 29 March 2012.
  2. 1 2 Bachorowski, Jo-Anne (April 1999). "Vocal Expression and Perception of Emotion". Current Directions in Psychological Science. 8 (2): 53–57. doi:10.1111/1467-8721.00013.
  3. 1 2 3 4 5 6 Sauter, Disa A.; Eisner, Frank; Calder, Andrew J.; Scott, Sophie K. (1 November 2010). "Perceptual cues in nonverbal vocal expressions of emotion". The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology. 63 (11): 2251–2272. doi:10.1080/17470211003721642.
  4. Pell, Marc D.; Kotz, Sonja A. (7 November 2011). "On the Time Course of Vocal Emotion Recognition". PLoS ONE. 6 (11): e27256. doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0027256. Retrieved 1 May 2012.
  5. 1 2 Berckmoes, Celine; Guy Vingerhoets (2004). "Neural Foundations of Emotional Speech Processing". Current Directions in Psychological Science. 13 (5): 182–185. doi:10.1111/j.0963-7214.2004.00303.x.
  6. Ryan, Melissa; Murray, Janice; Ruffman, Ted (Jan 2010). "Aging and the perception of emotion: Processing vocal emotions alone and with faces". Experimental Aging Research. 36 (1): 1–22. doi:10.1080/03610730903418372.
This article is issued from Wikipedia - version of the 9/12/2016. The text is available under the Creative Commons Attribution/Share Alike but additional terms may apply for the media files.