Emotional bias

An emotional bias is a distortion in cognition and decision making due to emotional factors.

That is, a person will be usually inclined

Those factors can be either individual and self-centered, or linked to interpersonal relationship or to group influence.

Effects

Its effects can be similar to those of a cognitive bias, it can even be considered as a subcategory of such biases. The specificity is that the cause lies in one's desires or fears, which divert the attention of the person, more than in one's reasoning.

Neuroscience experiments have shown how emotions and cognition, which are present in different areas of the human brain, interfere with each other in decision making process, resulting often in a primacy of emotions over reasoning[1]

Emotional bias might, for example, help explain the tendency towards over-optimism or over-pessimism, even when evidence for a more rational conclusion is available.

Decision making

Emotions have a small to large impact on the decisions we make depending on the type of emotion.[2] Some of the most influential emotions for decision-making are sadness, disgust, and guilt.[2] Anger differs the most from fear and sadness in both judgment and decision-making contexts.[2] Fear is associated with uncertainty, while sadness is associated with a perception that outcomes are due to the situation.[2] Angry decision-makers tend to make choices quickly and are unlikely to analyze their decisions.[3] Stress can play a role in decision-making. Acute stress can alter the response to moral dilemmas.[4] On the other hand, stress does not always alter everyday, moral decision-making.[5] One study looked at the role emotions play in adolescents’ moral decision-making. In a hypothetical, prosocial behavioral context, positively charged self-evaluative emotions most strongly predict moral choice.[6] In anti-social behaviors, negatively charged, critical emotions most strongly predict moral choice.[6] Regret and disappointment are emotions experienced after a decision. In some cases, regret has created a stronger desire to switch choices than disappointment.[7]

Emotions affect different types of decisions. Emotions have a strong influence on economic behavior and decision-making.[8] In some behavioral anomalies, certain emotions related to some tasks can have an increased impact.[9] In one experiment, researchers looked at what emotions manifest the disposition effect, where individuals sell winning shares and hold losing ones.[9] They found that elation for winners and regret for losers are necessary emotions that can cause the effect to occur.[9] In regards to patients making a medical decision, emotions and one’s motivational goals, play a part as well.[10] One study looked at the elements of coping behaviors.[10] The first two elements have to do with the need to control the cognitive and emotional elements of the health threat; the second pair of elements relate to the management of cognitive and emotional aspects of the decision itself.[10]

Brain damage can cause changes in normal decision-making processes. The amygdala is an area in the brain involved in emotion. Studies have found that patients with bilateral amygdala damage, which is damage in both hemispheres of the amygdala region in the brain, are deficient in decision-making.[11] When an initial choice is made in decision-making, the result of this choice has an emotional response, which is controlled by the amygdala.[11]

See also

References

  1. http://ccat.sas.upenn.edu/bmcr/2004/2004-04-19.html
  2. 1 2 3 4 Angie, A. D., Connelly, S., Waples, E. P., & Kligyte, V. (2011). The influence of discrete emotions on judgement and decision-making: A meta-analytic review. Cognition and Emotion, 25(8), 1393-1422. doi:10.1080/02699931.2010.550751 Retrieved on 10/4/12
  3. Lerner, J. S., & Tiedens, L. Z. (2006). Portrait of the angry decision maker: How appraisal tendencies shape anger's influence on cognition. Journal of Behavioral Decision Making, 19, 115-137. Retrieved on 10/4/12
  4. Youseff, F. F., Dookeeram, K., Basdeo, V., Francis, E., Doman, M., Mamed, D., & ... Legall, G. (2012). Stress alters personal moral decision-making. Psychoneuroendocrinology, 37(4), 491-498. doi:10.1016/j.psyneuen.2011.07.017 Retrieved on 10/4/12
  5. Starcke, K., Polzer, C., Wolf, O. T., & Brand, M. (2011). Does stress alter everyday moral decision-making?. Psychoneuroendocrinology, 36(2), 210-219. doi:10.1016/j.psyneuen.2010.07.010 Retrieved on 10/4/12
  6. 1 2 Krettenauer, T., Jia, F., & Mosleh, M. (2011). The role of emotion expectancies in adolescents’ moral decision making. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 108(2), 358-370. doi:10.1016/j.jecp.2010.08.014 Retrieved on 10/4/12
  7. Chua, H., Gonzalez, R., Taylor, S. F., Welsh, R. C., & Liberzon, I. (2009). Decision-related loss: Regret and disappointment. Neuroimage, 47(4), 2031-2040. doi:10.1016/j.neuroimage.2009.06.006 Retrieved on 10/4/12
  8. Weber, E., & Johnson, E. J. (2009). Mindful judgment and decision making. Annual Review of Psychology, 60, 53–85.
  9. 1 2 3 Summers, B., & Duxbury, D. (2012). Decision-dependent emotions and behavioral anomalies. Organizational Behavior and Human Decision Processes, 118(2), 226-238. doi:10.1016/j.obhdp.2012.03.004 Retrieved on 10/4/12
  10. 1 2 3 Power, T. E., Swartzman, L. C., & Robinson, J. W. (2011). Cognitive-emotional decision making (CEDM): A framework of patient medical decision making. Patient Education and Counseling, 83(2), 163-169. doi:10.1016/j.pec.2010.05.021 Retrieved on 10/4/12
  11. 1 2 Gupta, R., Koscik, T. R., Bechara, A., & Tranel, D. (2011). The amygdala and decision-making. Neuropsychologia, 49(4), 760-766. Retrieved on 10/4/12
This article is issued from Wikipedia - version of the 9/15/2016. The text is available under the Creative Commons Attribution/Share Alike but additional terms may apply for the media files.